Sunday, January 12, 2020

Three Days at the Teachers College Reading and Writing Project

Some take-aways from my recent three-day "institute" about student book groups at the Teachers College Reading and Writing Project. More on some of these later...

  • Stickies are a cool presentation tool
  • Notebooks are good, and other people are amazing at making their notebook pages look great, even if I'm not
  • Reading in class is still a revolutionary act
  • We can create the culture we believe in--but it takes real planning and work
    • TCRWP believes in a culture of reading, and they have a clear plan for how to create it
  • Reading in class for extended periods is very important
    • Teachers are incredibly resistant to this practice
    • The entire TCRWP "workshop method" can be seen as a clever, round-about way to try to make teachers comfortable with just letting their students read
  • * Our culture demands that we back our claims up with "research" or "science"
  • * It's hard to come up with good questions about literature
    • The "workshop" format the TCRWP promotes seems excellent (it minimizes, and maybe sharpens, direct instruction and group discussion, while maximizing time in class to actually read), but the questions they used in their mini-lessons just weren't that great (level 1 and 2 questions, mostly, if you use the 4QM framework, and, more importantly, not phrased very crisply) 
  • * It's hard to run a good class
    • The leader of our smaller group, when she was setting up our book groups, said, "It's important not to just put the books on a table at the back of the room and say, Okay, go find a book you like and get in groups" -- but then she did exactly that. Teaching is hard!
  • The street food in Morningside Heights is way better now than when I lived there in the late 90s.

Tuesday, January 7, 2020

Day 2: Two high points and a low point

I had an interesting day at the institute, and then I had a cup of coffee with my aunt, Hannah, who used to be an elementary school teacher and then a math coordinator, and we had an interesting talk about the culture of education. I was saying something about the reading wars and the way The Knowledge Gap vilified Lucy Calkins, and I said, “It’s funny, all educational debates follow the same basic structure, but I’m not sure I know what that structure is, exactly.”

“I know,” Hannah said: “taking two things that are both necessary and pitting them against each other."

She’s exactly right!

That was one high point. Another was when we had twenty minutes to just sit and read quietly, and our facilitator said, “Most importantly, you’ve got to protect your reading time!”

I agree, we have to protect our reading time, so I was less happy with the amount of time we spent looking at screens, images, and video. Ugh.

I guess I should have known this was coming after Mary Ehrenworth said yesterday, about using video in your classroom (I think she called it, in a strange phrase, “digital read-alouds”): “This is a game-changer,” and added, “You should be using it in your classrooms as much as humanly possible.”

Yikes!

Are educational researchers more "analytical" than teachers? I doubt it.

Educational research has a lot of problems--statistical incompetence, a tendency to leap to generalizations, and the Kuhnian inability of all scientists, even when the data is decisive, to "renounce the paradigm that has led them into crisis", among many others.  Why these weaknesses?  Is it because many of the people doing this research used to be teachers, and teaching makes you less "analytical"?  No! This is not the reason!  Teachers can be analytical too!

Stanford's David Labaree
David Labaree, a sociologist by training and a longtime Ed School professor, is always provocative and interesting.  I've read a number of his articles, and I like what he's written about statistics in Ed research (he suggests Educational researchers use statistics because, working in the least respected field in the academy, and perhaps the only field that is both "soft" and "applied", they're grasping for authority), and I am very interested in his work on education as a public good (he uses Hirschman's distinction between exit and voice as a helpful lens, and he makes useful distinctions among the different purposes schooling can have, from educating citizens to training workers, both clearly public goods, to allowing for individual social mobility and competitive advantage, which Labaree sees as a private good, though I think this too has a public good aspect).  I'm also interested in his writing about ed schools, but that writing seems a bit more muddled to me.  Perhaps it's the subject.

Are practical-minded teachers to blame for the problems with educational research?
Labaree suggests, in a 2003 article, that educational researchers are more "analytical" than teachers. He claims that one of the tensions in Ed schools is the cultural shift that doctoral Education students must make when they enter Ed school and have to shift from the practical exigencies of teaching to the scholarly culture of being an educational researcher.  It seems to me, however, that a more important problem is the way educational researchers shift from being analytical and scholarly in their professional papers to being polemical and unscholarly in their popular writing and their policy advocacy.

The 2003 article I'm focusing on here appeared in Educational Researcher, a publication of the American Education Research Association.  The article discusses the cultural initiation undergone by educational researchers-in-training. Labaree claims that these graduate students, who are nearly always former teachers, must make a difficult transition from the culture of the practitioner to the culture of the scholar.  According to Labaree, these students' reluctance to think in analytical, scholarly ways "leads students in education doctoral programs to shift the discourse about educational issues from what is to what should be, looking for practical solutions before explaining the problem." In other words, Labaree seems to be blaming the tendency of educational research to be always chasing after the next fad in educational practice on this cultural divide.  As Labaree puts it:

The idea is to pick an intervention that promises to improve education—a new teaching technique, curriculum approach, instructional technology, reform effort, or administrative structure—and study it in practice. The desired outcome is that the intervention works rather well, and the function of the study is to document this and suggest how the approach could be improved in the future. This often leads to an approach to scholarship (and eventually to a kind of scholarly literature) that is relentlessly, unrealistically, sometimes comically optimistic—one that suggests that there is an implementable answer to every educational problem and that help is always on the way.

I love this focus on the way education research often puts the cart before the horse, and I think Labaree's urging of modesty on the often-hubristic educational researchers is right on.  In another article Labaree calls on education scholars to adopt the Hippocratic "First, do no harm" as a slogan, which strikes me as exactly right.  For the first few years of my teaching career, that slogan was my daily mantra.  Nevertheless, I think Labaree is putting the blame in the wrong place here.  Rather than blame first the culture of the working practitioner for the intellectual hubris of ed research, I would put most of the responsibility on the culture of the scholars and professors.

Part of the problem is one that Labaree himself has written about extensively: education research is the lowliest academic pursuit.  Because education research is marginal and low-prestige, ed school professors have chips on their shoulders, are even more susceptible to political and cultural influence than scholars in more prestigious disciplines, and are statistically incompetent. But the real problem is that this is an "applied" social science, and applied social sciences struggle mightily because they are up against an essentially impossible task. The old poem I quoted yesterday is relevant again:

No known way of human seeing
Can clearly see the human being.

If social science is a well-nigh impossible task, and if Doctoral students in Education are insecure because they see legions of doctoral students in Economics about to take their jobs, then why should they be any more scrupulous about curbing the many types of researcher bias than any other social scientist? For the truth is that social science has been for years in the throes of a crisis: a study published in Nature found that more than a third of a group of peer-reviewed social science papers published in top journals (Nature and Science) couldn't be replicated. Researchers in other social sciences are, clearly, also swayed by the hope of finding meaningful and applicable results.

On the other hand, teachers can be and are often quite analytical, and are in fact more truly scientific than many social scientists. One key element of science is maintaining an open, skeptical, humble mind, and few people are more humble in my experience than working teachers.

Two recent critiques of the education establishment, and teacher practice, are Natalie Wexler's book, The Knowledge Gap, and Emily Hanford's many radio stories, op-eds and film about the "science" of reading. Both Wexler and Hanford are reporters who base their critiques on supposed "science" that they got from academic researchers (Mark Seidenberg in Hanford's case, and Daniel Willingham (mostly) in Wexler's case); and both Wexler and Hanford cast Lucy Calkins, an ed school bigwig, as their main villain, with the teachers in the role of mostly unwitting dupes who need to be informed that they are doing everything wrong. In these feuds, it is absolutely not the ed school people (and of course it's not the teachers), who are "relentlessly, unrealistically, sometimes comically optimistic."  Instead, the people who most wildly overstate their case and pretend that "there is an implementable answer to every educational problem" are not only the journalists, no doubt incentivized by the pressure of coming up with a good story, but also the scientists themselves. After all, the hubristic subtitle of Seidenberg's book is "How We Read, Why So Many Can't, and What Can Be Done About It", and Willingham's book has a whole table of implementable solutions. In both of these cases, the problems are ones that Labaree points out, but the fault lies not in teachers, nor in ed school.

The next question, for me, is how much any of this really matters. Fortunately, none of it is probably all that significant. Even what I was worrying about today--the crazy numbers of screens we have these days in English classrooms that are supposed to be helping kids get better at reading and writing--probably doesn't matter all that much. But more on that issue later.

Monday, January 6, 2020

Two Cultures: “Science” vs. “Research” (Reflections on Day 1 of the TCRWP Institute)


I’m still curious why this is called an “Institute.” Having spent my undergraduate years at the world’s most famous “Institute,” I may be particularly sensitive to the word, but it’s also, as Stephen King knows, just objectively uncanny.

I didn’t learn anything about that word, but I did get a better picture of how people here at the Teachers College Reading and Writing Project use the word “science.” The answer is: they don’t.

I didn’t hear the word “science” once all day. On the other hand, I did hear, quite often, the word “research.” Several times, presenters talked explicitly and at some length about “research”—“Reading Research” or “Non-Fiction Research” or “research on the reciprocity of writing and reading.”

Mary Ehrenworth, the lead organizer of the Institute, gave an hour-long talk over half of which was an overview of the “research” supporting her approach. Interestingly, she presented this research not as specific studies or types of inquiry or categories of interest within the field, but as individual researchers within the field. When talking about “Reading Research,” for instance, she listed six particular scholars and discussed one or more of their books. Organizing the talk this way was interesting, subtly prioritizing both the book (over the “peer-reviewed paper” that is the lodestar of the “Science” folks) and also the human. Organizing a discussion of the research by researchers is a way of valuing the human and cultural nature of educational scholarship, and, the “science” people would say, completely ignoring the “scientific” nature of it.

I remember Mark Seidenberg--a cognitive scientist who is so annoyed by the TCRWP’s insufficient embrace of the “science” supporting systematic phonics that he more or less implies that the TCRWP’s Lucy Calkins is herself the reason Americans can’t read (ironically humanizing what is after all supposed to be an objective debate)—I remember Mark Seidenberg saying that there were two distinct cultures, and that Calkins and her ilk (the whole literacy establishment, including Allington and Beers and others) were part of an “educational culture” which was distinct from the “scientific culture” to which Seidenberg belonged. In Seidenberg’s view, the reason Americans lag in reading (if they do) is that educational culture is interested in culture and socialization and wholly uninterested in science. Based on what I saw today, I think Seidenberg is half right. The people here are not really interested in science.

Where I differ from Seidenberg is that I think he vastly overestimates the extent to which the “science” of education is settled or helpful. Part of this may be that the people here are focused on all of K-12 education, while he is for various reasons more focused on the basics of learning to read. Part of this is because scholarship around education is itself anxious about its status as “science” (you can see this even in the work of a researcher cited twice today, John Hattie, whose work claims way more than it delivers). But a lot of it is that, as an old poem has it, the human and social “sciences” are still in their infancies:

     No known way of human seeing
     Can clearly see the human being.

In any case, I'm looking forward to Day 2!

Off to Anti-Science HQ...

In about five minutes I'm going to join some colleagues and walk up to an "institute" at Lucy Calkins's Reading and Writing Project at Teachers College. Calkins has been under fire more than ever recently; she seems to be the focal point around which circle the literacy pundits who harp most noisily about the importance of "science" as the foundation of all educational practice. Natalie Wexler's popular book, The Knowledge Gap, casts Calkins as its villain, and the various pro-phonics people in the reading wars--journalist Emily Hanford and cognitive scientist Mark Seidenberg are the ones I've noticed most--also love to hate on Calkins. Seidenberg recently wrote a blog post with the amazing title, "This is Why We Don't Have Better Readers", as if Calkins were single-handedly holding back the nation's youth. And who knows, maybe she is.

Having never gone to Ed School, I'm curious to see what I'll find. I'll write more later...